Scientific community

Descriptive and normative



On heuristic mechanism and institutional design for communities of scientific practice for the common property resource that is human knowledge. Sociology of science, in other words. How do diverse underfunded teams manage to advance truth with their weird prestige economy despite the many pitfalls of publication filters and such? What is effective here in designing communities, practice and social norms? How much communication is too much? How much iconoclasm is right to defeat groupthink and foster the spread of good ideas?

A place to file questions like this, in other words (O’Connor and Wu 2021):

Diversity of practice is widely recognized as crucial to scientific progress. If all scientists perform the same tests in their research, they might miss important insights that other tests would yield. If all scientists adhere to the same theories, they might fail to explore other options which, in turn, might be superior. But the mechanisms that lead to this sort of diversity can also generate epistemic harms when scientific communities fail to reach swift consensus on successful theories. In this paper, we draw on extant literature using network models to investigate diversity in science. We evaluate different mechanisms from the modeling literature that can promote transient diversity of practice, keeping in mind ethical and practical constraints posed by real epistemic communities. We ask: what are the best ways to promote the right amount of diversity of practice in such communities?

References

Agassi, Joseph. 1974. “The Logic of Scientific Inquiry.” Synthese 26: 498–514. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00883107.
Alon, Uri. 2009. “How to Choose a Good Scientific Problem.” Molecular Cell 35 (6): 726–28. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.molcel.2009.09.013.
Arbesman, Samuel, and Nicholas A Christakis. 2011. “Eurekometrics: Analyzing the Nature of Discovery.” PLoS Comput Biol 7 (6): –1002072. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002072.
Azoulay, Pierre, Christian Fons-Rosen, and Joshua S. Graff Zivin. 2015. “Does Science Advance One Funeral at a Time?” Working Paper 21788. National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w21788.
Bhattacharya, Jay, and Mikko Packalen. 2020. “Stagnation and Scientific Incentives.” Working Paper 26752. National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.3386/w26752.
Gasparyan, Armen Yuri, Alexey N. Gerasimov, Alexander A. Voronov, and George D. Kitas. 2015. “Rewarding Peer Reviewers: Maintaining the Integrity of Science Communication.” Journal of Korean Medical Science 30 (4): 360–64. https://doi.org/10.3346/jkms.2015.30.4.360.
Greenberg, Steven A. 2009. “How Citation Distortions Create Unfounded Authority: Analysis of a Citation Network.” BMJ 339 (July): b2680. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b2680.
Healy, Kieran. 2015. “The Performativity of Networks.” European Journal of Sociology 56 (02): 175–205. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003975615000107.
Ioannidis, John P. 2005. “Why Most Published Research Findings Are False.” PLoS Medicine 2 (8): –124. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.0020124.
Jan, Zeeshan. n.d. “Recognition and Reward System for Peer-Reviewers,” 9.
Kearns, Hugh, and Maria Gardiner. 2011. “The Care and Maintenance of Your Adviser.” Nature 469 (7331): 570–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nj7331-570a.
Lakatos, Imre. 1980. The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes: Volume 1 : Philosophical Papers. Cambridge University Press.
Merrifield, Michael R, and Donald G Saari. 2009. “Telescope Time Without Tears: A Distributed Approach to Peer Review.” Astronomy & Geophysics 50 (4): 4.16–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-4004.2009.50416.x.
Nissen, Silas B., Tali Magidson, Kevin Gross, and Carl T. Bergstrom. 2016. “Publication Bias and the Canonization of False Facts.” arXiv:1609.00494 [physics, Stat], September. http://arxiv.org/abs/1609.00494.
O’Connor, Cailin, and Jingyi Wu. 2021. “How Should We Promote Transient Diversity in Science?” MetaArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31222/osf.io/w3xc5.
Rekdal, Ole Bjørn. 2014. “Academic Urban Legends.” Social Studies of Science 44 (4): 638–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/0306312714535679.
Robbins, Lionel. 1932. An Essay on the Nature and Significance of Economic Science. Macmillan.
Rzhetsky, Andrey, Jacob G. Foster, Ian T. Foster, and James A. Evans. 2015. “Choosing Experiments to Accelerate Collective Discovery.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 112 (47): 14569–74. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509757112.
Spranzi, Marta. 2004. “Galileo and the Mountains of the Moon: Analogical Reasoning, Models and Metaphors in Scientific Discovery.” Journal of Cognition and Culture 4 (3): 451–83. https://doi.org/10.1163/1568537042484904.
Stove, David Charles. 1982. Popper and After: Four Modern Irrationalists. Pergamon.
Suppes, Patrick. 2002. Representation and Invariance of Scientific Structures. CSLI Publications.
Thagard, Paul. 1993. “Societies of Minds: Science as Distributed Computing.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Modern Physics 24: 49.
———. 1994. “Mind, Society, and the Growth of Knowledge.” Philosophy of Science 61.
———. 1997. “Collaborative Knowledge.” Noûs 31 (2): 242–61.
———. 2005. “How to Be a Successful Scientist.” Scientific and Technological Thinking, 159–71.
———. 2007. “Coherence, Truth, and the Development of Scientific Knowledge.” Philosophy of Science 74: 28–47. https://doi.org/10.1086/520941.
Thagard, Paul, and Abninder Litt. 2008. “Models of Scientific Explanation.” In The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Thagard, Paul, and Jing Zhu. 2003. “Acupuncture, Incommensurability, and Conceptual Change.” Intentional Conceptual Change, 79–102.
Thurner, Stefan, and Rudolf Hanel. 2010. “Peer-Review in a World with Rational Scientists: Toward Selection of the Average.”
Vazire, Simine. 2017. “Our Obsession with Eminence Warps Research.” Nature News 547 (7661): 7. https://doi.org/10.1038/547007a.
Weisbuch, Gérard, Guillaume Deffuant, Frédéric Amblard, and Jean-Pierre Nadal. 2002. “Meet, Discuss, and Segregate!” Complexity 7 (3): 55–63. https://doi.org/10.1002/cplx.10031.
Weng, L, A Flammini, A Vespignani, F Menczer, L Weng, A Flammini, A Vespignani, and F Menczer. 2012. “Competition Among Memes in a World with Limited Attention.” Scientific Reports 2. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep00335.
Wible, James R. 1998. Economics of Science. Routledge.
Woodley, Lou, and Katie Pratt. 2020. “The CSCCE Community Participation Model – A Framework to Describe Member Engagement and Information Flow in STEM Communities,” August. https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.3997802.
Yarkoni, Tal. 2019. “The Generalizability Crisis.” Preprint. PsyArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jqw35.
Zimmer, Carl. 2020. “How You Should Read Coronavirus Studies, or Any Science Paper.” The New York Times, June 1, 2020, sec. Science. https://www.nytimes.com/article/how-to-read-a-science-study-coronavirus.html.

No comments yet. Why not leave one?

GitHub-flavored Markdown & a sane subset of HTML is supported.